Praising Young Children for Being Smart Promotes Cheating
Kang Lee is a brilliant researcher who is one of the very few professors at OISE who isn't a crazed Marxist. His early childhood psychology research is indeed groundbreaking, however it appears OISE never incorporates its results into their other programs.
Praise is one of the most commonly used forms of
reward. It is convenient, is nearly effortless, and makes
the recipient feel good. However, praising children for
being smart carries unintended consequences: It can
undermine their achievement motivation in a way that
praising their effort or performance does not (Cimpian,
Arce, Markman, & Dweck, 2007; Kamins & Dweck,
1999; Mueller & Dweck, 1998; see Dweck, 2007). In this
study, we investigated whether the negative consequences
of praising children for being smart extend to
the moral domain, by encouraging cheating.
There is some prior work suggesting that evaluative
feedback can influence children’s moral behaviors (Fu,
Heyman, Qian, Guo, & Lee, 2016; Mueller & Dweck,
1998; Zhao, Heyman, Chen, & Lee, 2017). Telling 5-yearolds
(but not younger children) that they have a reputation
for being good leads to a reduction in their
cheating, presumably because they are interested in
maintaining this reputation (Fu et al., 2016). We propose
that telling children that they are smart, a form of
ability praise, may have the opposite effect by motivating
them to cheat to appear smarter. In a study consistent
with this possibility, Mueller and Dweck (1998)
found that 10-year-olds exaggerated how well they had
performed after receiving ability praise. However, little
is known about whether ability praise can influence
young children’s moral behavior. The present research
addressed this question by comparing the effects of
ability and performance praise on preschool children’s
cheating.
Method
Participants were 300 preschool children in eastern
China: one hundred fifty 3-year-olds (age range = 3.08 to 4.00 years, M = 3.62, SD = 0.27; 71 boys, 79 girls)
and one hundred fifty 5-year-olds (age range = 5.01 to
6.00 years, M = 5.38, SD = 0.33; 78 boys, 72 girls). To
measure cheating, we used a version of a wellestablished
peeking paradigm (see Heyman, Fu, Lin,
Qian, & Lee, 2015), in which an experimenter hides a
playing card (with a number from 3 to 9, excluding 6)
behind a barrier and children guess whether it is greater
or less than 6. The children are told that they can win
a prize if they guess correctly on at least three of the
six trials.
The session began with a practice trial in which the
children were told that they had guessed correctly. They
were then randomly assigned to three conditions (50
children in each condition): In the ability condition,
children were told, “You are so smart.” In the performance
condition, they were told, “You did very well
this time.” In the baseline condition, no praise was
given.
The real guessing game, which was identical across
the three conditions, followed this practice trial. On
each trial, the children were instructed not to peek.
Unbeknownst to them, the game was rigged to ensuresuccess on two of the first five trials and failure on three...
No comments:
Post a Comment